PL EN


Preferencje help
Widoczny [Schowaj] Abstrakt
Liczba wyników
2015 | 9 | nr 3 | 337--351
Tytuł artykułu

Investors' Expertise, Personality Traits and Susceptibility to Behavioral Biases in the Decision Making Process

Treść / Zawartość
Warianty tytułu
Języki publikacji
EN
Abstrakty
EN
The aim of this paper is to investigate the degree of susceptibility to behavioral biases (the certainty effect, the sunk cost fallacy, and mental accounting) among people of various levels of expertise in market investments and to determine whether this susceptibility is correlated with certain personality traits (impulsivity, venturesomeness, and empathy). The study included 200 participants: 100 retail investors who regularly invest in the Warsaw Stock Exchange and 100 students of the Warsaw School of Economics who are casually involved in investing. In this study, employing a survey methodology, we conducted a laboratory experiment that allowed us to isolate behavioral biases and personality traits and measure their influence on investors' decision-making processes. The participants filled out questionnaires containing two parts: 1) three situational exercises, which assessed susceptibility to behavioral biases, and 2) the Impulsivity, Venturesomeness, Empathy Questionnaire (IVE) Questionnaire which measures three personality traits (impulsivity, venturesomeness, and empathy). Statistical analyses demonstrated that susceptibility to behavioral biases depends on the level of expertise in market investing such that expertise increases susceptibility to behavioral biases. Some personality traits influenced the participants' likelihood of displaying these biases. (original abstract)
Rocznik
Tom
9
Numer
Strony
337--351
Opis fizyczny
Twórcy
  • University of Finance and Management in Warsaw, Poland
autor
  • Warsaw School of Economics, Poland
  • Warsaw School of Economics, Poland
Bibliografia
  • Agnew J.R. (2006). Do behavioral biases vary across individuals? Evidence from individual level 401(k) data. Journal of Financial and Quantitative Analysis, 41 (4), 939-962.
  • Akerlof G.A., & Schiller R.J. (2009). How Human Psychology Drives the Economy and Why It Matters for Global Capitalism. Ewing, NJ: Princeton University Press.
  • Alessi S.M., & Petry N.M. (2003). Pathological gambling severity is associated with impulsivity in a delay discounting procedure. Behavioural Processes, 64 (3), 345-354.
  • Arkes H., & Blumer C. (1985). The psychology of sunk cost. Organisational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 35, 124-140.
  • Baddeley M., Burke C., Tobler P. & Schulz W. (2010). Impacts of Personality on Herding in Financial Decision-Making (Working Paper No. 1006). Faculty of Economics, University of Cambridge.
  • Belcher L.J. (2010). Prior perceptions, personality characteristics and portfolio preferences among fund managers: an experimental analysis. Journal of Behavioral Finance, 11 (4), 239-248.
  • Benos A. (1998). Aggressiveness and survival of overconfident traders. Journal of Financial Markets, 1 (3-4), 353-383.
  • Bernstein P.L. (2007). Capital Ideas Evolving. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons, Inc.
  • Borghans L., Duckworth A., Heckman J., & Weel B. (2008). The economics and psychology of personality traits. Journal of Human Resources, 43 (4), 972-1059.
  • Braun P., & Yaniv I. (1992). A case study of expert judgment: economists' probabilities versus baserate model forecasts. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 5 (3), 217-231.
  • Camerer C.F., & Johnson E.J. (1997). The process performance paradox in expert judgment: how can experts know so much and predict so badly. In: W.M. Goldstein & R.M. Hogarth (Eds.), Research on judgment and decision making: Currents, connections, and controversies (pp. 342-364). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
  • Camgoz M., Karan B., & Ergeneli A. (2011). Relationship between the Big-Five Personality and the financial performance of fund managers. Current Topics in Management, 15, 137-152.
  • Chira I., Adams M., & Thornton B. (2008). Behavioral bias within the decision making process. Journal of Business & Economics Research, 6 (8), 11-20.
  • Connolly T., & Zeelenberg M. (2002). Regret in decision making. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 11 (6), 212-216.
  • Corter J., & Chen Y. (2006). Do investment risk tolerance attitudes predict portfolio risk? Journal of Business and Psychology, 20 (3), 369-381.
  • Costa P.T., & McCrae R.R. (1985). The NEO Personality Inventory manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.
  • Coval J.D., & Shumway T. (2005). Do behavioral biases affect prices? The Journal of Finance, 60 (1), 1-34.
  • De Bondt W., & Thaler R. (1987). Further evidence on investor overreaction and stock market seasonality. The Journal of Finance, 42 (3), 557-580.
  • De Bondt W., & Makhija A. (1988). Throwing good money after bad? Nuclear power plant investment decisions and the relevance of sunk costs. Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization, 10 (2), 173-199.
  • Dror I., Katona M., & Mungur K. (1998). Age Differences in Decision Making: To Take a Risk or Not? Gerontology, 44 (2), 67-71.
  • Durand R.B., Newby R., & Sanghani J. (2008). An Intimate Portrait of the Individual Investor. Journal of Behavioral Finance 9 (4), 193-208.
  • Eysenck S.B.G., & Eysenck H.J. (1978). Impulsiveness and venturesomeness: Their position in a dimensional system of personality description. Psychological Reports, 43 (3), 1247-1255.
  • Eysenck H.J., & Eysenck S.B.G. (2006). Manual for the Eysenck Personality Scales (EPS Adult) including EPQ-Revised, EPQ- Short Scale, IVE Questionnaire. London, UK: Hodder and Stoughton Educational.
  • Fama E.F. (1970). Efficient capital markets: A review of theory and empirical work. Journal of Finance, 25 (2), 383-417.
  • Fama E.F. (1991). Efficient capital markets II. Journal of Finance, 46 (5), 1575-1617.
  • Freedman, D. (2009). Statistical Models: Theory and Practice (2nd ed.). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
  • Friedman D., Pommerenke K., Lukose R., Milam G., & Huberman B.A. (2007). Searching for the sunk cost fallacy. Experimental Economics, 10 (1), 79-104.
  • Garvey R., & Murphy A. (2008). Are professional traders too slow to realize their losses? Financial Analysts Journal, 60 (4), 35-43.
  • Haigh M., & List J. (2005). Do professional traders exhibit myopic loss aversion? An experimental analysis. The Journal of Finance, 60 (1), 523-534.
  • Hayaki J., Anderson B., & Stein M. (2006). Sexual risk behaviors among substance users: relationship to impulsivity. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 20 (3), 328-332.
  • Hopfensitz A., & Wranik T. (2009). How to Adapt to Changing Markets: Experience and Personality in a Repeated Investment Game (Working Paper No. 122). Toulouse School of Economics.
  • Jadlow J., & Mowen J. (2010). Comparing the Traits of Stock Market Investors and Gamblers. Journal of Behavioral Finance, 11 (2), 67-81.
  • Kahneman D., & Tversky A. (1973). On the psychology of prediction. Psychological Review, 80 (4), 237-251.
  • Kahneman D., & Tversky A. (1979). Prospect theory: An analysis of decisions under risk. Economoterica, 47 (2), 263-292.
  • Kahneman D., & Tversky A. (1984). Choices, values and frames. American Psychologist, 39 (4), 341-350.
  • Kahneman D. (2012). Thinking fast and slow. New York, NY: Penguin Books Ltd.
  • Krems J., & Zierer C. (1994). Are experts immune to cognitive bias? Dependence of "confirmation bias" on specialist knowledge. Zeitschrift für experimentelle und angewandte Psychologie, 41 (1), 98-115.
  • Markowitz H.M. (1952). Portfolio selection. Journal of finance, 7 (1), 77-91.
  • Mayfield C., Perdue G., & Wooten K. (2008). Investment management and personality type. Financial Services Review, 17 (3), 219-236.
  • McAfee R.P., Mialon H.M., & Mialon S.H. (2010). Do sunk costs matter? Economic Inquiry, 48 (2), 323-336.
  • Mishra S., Lalumiere M.L., & Williams R.J. (2010). Gambling as a form of risk-taking: individual differences in personality, risk-accepting attitudes, and behavioral preferences for risk. Personality and Individual Differences 49 (6), 616-621.
  • Odean T. (1998). Are investors reluctant to realize their losses. Journal of Finance, 53 (5), 1775-1798.
  • Pompian M., & Longo J. (2004). A new paradigm for practical application of behavioral finance: creating investment programs based on personality type and gender to produce better investment outcomes. The Journal of Wealth Management, 7 (2), 9-15.
  • Roszkowski M., & Grable J. (2005). Estimating risk tolerance: the degree of accuracy and the paramorphic representations of the estimate. Financial Counseling and Planning, 16 (2), 29-47.
  • Rzeszutek M., Czerwonka M. (2011). Analiza zachowań inwestycyjnych inwestorów giełdowych i studentów kierunków ekonomicznych i humanistycznych z perspektywy finansów behawioralnych [Analysis of investment behawior of stock market investors and economics and humanities students from the perspective of behavioral finance]. Studia i Prace Kolegium Zarządzania i Finansów Szkoły Głównej Handlowej, 107, 28-44.
  • Sadi R., Asl H.G., Rostami M., Gholipour A., & Gholipour F. (2011). Behavioral finance: the explanation of investors' personality and perceptual biases effects on financial decisions. International Journal of Economics and Finance, 3 (5), 234-241.
  • Sjoberg L., & Engelberg E. (2009). Attitudes to economic risk taking, sensation seeking and values of business students specializing in finance. Journal of Behavioral Finance, 10 (1), 33-43.
  • Stephan E. (1998, June). Heuristics and biases in decision making: the role of incentives, ability, and expertise. Paper presented at research Seminar held at the Erasmus University Rotterdam.
  • Sultana S.T., & Pardhasaradhi S. (2010). An empirical investigation of the relation between risk tolerance and socioeconomic characteristics of individual investors. Advances in Management, 4 (10), 60-66.
  • Szyszka A. (2007). Wycena papierów wartościowych na rynku kapitałowym w świetle finansów behawioralnych [A behavioral finanse perspective on valuation of securities on the capital market]. Poznań: Wydawnictwo Akademii Ekonomicznej w Poznaniu.
  • Szyszka A., Zielonka P. (2007). The Disposition Effect Demonstrated on IPO Trading Volume. Journal of Behavioral Finance, 4 (3), 40-48.
  • Szyszka A. (2010). Preferences and belief based models. In: H.K. Baker & J. Nofsinger (Eds.), Behavioral Finance: Investors, Corporations, and Markets, (pp. 351 - 372). Hoboken, NJ: J. Wiley & Sons.
  • Tetlock P. (2005). Expert Political Judgment: How Good Is It? How Can We Know? Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
  • Thaler R. (1999). Mental accounting matters. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 12 (3), 183-206.
  • Todd P.M., & Gigerenzer G. (2003). Bounding rationality to the world. Journal of Economic Psychology, 24 (2), 143-165.
  • Tyszka T., & Zielonka P. (2002). Expert judgments: financial analysts vs. weather forecasters. Journal of Psychology and Financial Markets, 3 (3), 152-160.
  • Von Neumann J., & Morgenstern O. (1944). Theory of games and economic behavior. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
  • Verma R., Baklaci H., & Soydemir G. (2008). The impact of rational and irrational sentiments of individual and institutional investors on DJIA and S&P500 index returns. Applied Financial Economics, 18 (16), 1303-1317.
  • Weller J.A., & Tikir A. (2011). Predicting domainspecific risk taking with the HEXACO personality structure. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 24 (2), 180-201.
  • Winnett A., & Lewis A. (1994). Household accounts, mental accounts and savings behaviour: Some old economics rediscovered? Journal of Economic Psychology, 16 (4), 431 - 448.
Typ dokumentu
Bibliografia
Identyfikatory
Identyfikator YADDA
bwmeta1.element.ekon-element-000171503161

Zgłoszenie zostało wysłane

Zgłoszenie zostało wysłane

Musisz być zalogowany aby pisać komentarze.
JavaScript jest wyłączony w Twojej przeglądarce internetowej. Włącz go, a następnie odśwież stronę, aby móc w pełni z niej korzystać.