PL EN


Preferencje help
Widoczny [Schowaj] Abstrakt
Liczba wyników
2020 | 28 | nr 4 | 67--85
Tytuł artykułu

No Trust vs. Some Trust in a Game Framed as Trust or Investment: Avoiding the Distrustor

Autorzy
Warianty tytułu
Języki publikacji
EN
Abstrakty
EN
Purpose: The study aimed at examining how observing a person showing no trust or some trust, in interaction framed as a trust or an investment, influences two variables: anticipated dissatisfaction from cooperating with the trustor and willingness to avoid such a person in future cooperative tasks. Additionally, the perception of the trustor and anticipated feelings in the role of the trustee were analyzed.

Methodology: A scenario describing the trust game framed as trust or investment was used in the study. Participants (N = 166) were randomly assigned to one of the four conditions: 2 (amount sent in the game: zero vs. one-tenth of the endowment) x 2 (game framing: trust vs. investment) and after reading the scenario made three types of evaluations: (1) their feelings in the position of the trustee from the scenario; (2) the trustor's sociability, morality, and competence and (3) anticipated dissatisfaction from cooperating with the trustor in the future, and finally (4) willingness to avoid the trustor in future interactions.

Results: Interaction effects were found for evaluations of anticipated dissatisfaction from cooperating with the trustor and willingness to avoid the trustor in future interactions. Observing sending nothing in the trust game framed as trust, rather than investment, resulted in stronger anticipated dissatisfaction from cooperating in the future with the trustor, and stronger willingness to avoid such a person as a partner for cooperation. Moreover, independent of the framing of the game, in condition of no trust, participants reported stronger negative feeling when imagining themselves in the role of the trustee and perceived the trustor as less sociable and less competent. Surprisingly, the perception of trustor's morality did not differ across the conditions.

Conclusions: Observing distrust, particularly when the interaction is interpreted in social terms as trust, rather than in economic ones as non-investing, with all other aspects of the situation the same, results in different attitude towards the distrustor on the side of observers. Distrust, in comparison to just non-investing, evokes in observers a stronger propensity to avoid such the distrustor as a partner for cooperation and lowers their anticipated satisfaction from cooperation with him/her. (original abstract)
Słowa kluczowe
Rocznik
Tom
28
Numer
Strony
67--85
Opis fizyczny
Twórcy
autor
  • Kozminski University, Warsaw, Poland
Bibliografia
  • Baumard, N. (2010). Has punishment played a role in the evolution of cooperation? A critical review. Mind & Society, 9(2), 171-192. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11299-010-0079-9.
  • Baumard, N. (2011). Punishment is not a group adaptation. Mind & Society, 10(1), 1-26. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11299-010-0080-3.
  • Berg, J., Dickhaut, J., and McCabe, K. (1995). Trust, reciprocity and social history, Games and Economic Behavior, 10, 122-142. https://doi.org/10.1006/game.1995.1027.
  • Bicchieri, C., Xiao, E., and Muldoon, R. (2011). Trustworthiness is a social norm, but trusting is not. Politics, Philosophy & Economics, 10(2), 170-187. https://doi.org/10.1177/1470594X10387260.
  • Camerer, C. (2003). Behavioral game theory: Experiments on strategic interaction. Princeton: Princeton University Press.
  • Clark, M.S., and Mills, J. (1993). The difference between communal and exchange relationships: What it is and is not. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 19, 684-692.
  • Dunning, D., Anderson, J. E., Schlösser, T., Ehlebracht, D., and Fetchenhauer, D. (2014). Trust at zero acquaintance: More a matter of respect than expectation of reward. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 107(1), 122-141. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0036673.
  • Dunning, D., Fetchenhauer, D., and Schloesser, T. (2016). The Psychology of Respect A case study of How Behavioral norms regulate human action. In: A. Elliot (ed.), Advances in motivation science (Vol. 3, pp. 1-34). New York, NY: Elsevier. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/bs.adms.2015.12.003.
  • Dunning, D., Fetchenhauer, D., and Schlösser, T.M. (2012). Trust as a social and emotional act: Noneconomic considerations in trust behavior q. Journal of Economic Psychology, 33(3), 686-694. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.joep.2011.09.005.
  • Eiser, J.R. and Bhavnani, K. (1974). The effect of situational meaning on the behaviour of subjects in the Prisoner's Dilemma Game. European Journal of Social Psychology, 4(1), 93-97.
  • Evans, A.M., and van de Calseyde, P. (2017). The reputational consequences of generalized trust. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 44, 492-507. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167217742886.
  • Feinberg, M., Willer, R., and Schultz, M. (2014). Gossip and ostracism promote cooperation in groups. Psychological Science, 25, 656-664. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797613510184.
  • Gerlach, P., and Jaeger, B. (2016). Another frame, another game? Explaining framing effects in economic games. Norms, Actions, Games, (June), 1-10. https://doi.org/10.17605/OSF.IO/AB5YP
  • Graham, J., Haidt, J., Koleva, S., Motyl, M., Iyer, R., Wojcik, S. P., and Ditto, P.H. (2013). Moral foundations theory: The pragmatic validity of moral pluralism. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 47, 55-130.
  • Johnson, N.D., and Mislin, A.A. (2011). Trust games: A meta-analysis. Journal of Economic Psychology, 32(5), 865-889. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.joep.2011.05.007.
  • Kay, A.C., and Ross, L. (2003). The perceptual push: The interplay of implicit cues and explicit situational construals on behavioral intentions in the Prisoner's Dilemma. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 39, 634-643. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0022-1031(03)00057-X.
  • Kemper, N.S., and Newheiser, A.-K. (2017). To Confront or to Avoid. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 194855061772283. https://doi.org/10.1177/1948550617722831.
  • Krueger, J.I., Massey, A.L., and DiDonato, T.E. (2008). A matter of trust: From social preferences to the strategic adherence to social norms. Negotiation and Conflict Management Research, 1, 31-52.
  • Liberman, V., Samuels, S.M., and Ross, L. (2004). The name of the game: Predictive power of reputations versus situational labels in determining Prisoner's Dilemma game moves. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30, 1175-1185. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167204264004.
  • Liddell, T.M., and Kruschke, J.K. (2014). Ostracism and fines in a public goods game with accidental contributions: The importance of punishment type. Judgment and Decision Making, 9(6), 523-547.
  • Martin, J., Young, L., and McAuliffe, K. (2019). The psychology of partner choice. https://psyarxiv.com/weqhz/.
  • Wu, J., Balliet, D., and Van Lange, P.A.M. (2016). Reputation, gossip, and human cooperation. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 10, 350-364. https://doi.org/10.1111/spc3.12255.
Typ dokumentu
Bibliografia
Identyfikatory
Identyfikator YADDA
bwmeta1.element.ekon-element-000171612009

Zgłoszenie zostało wysłane

Zgłoszenie zostało wysłane

Musisz być zalogowany aby pisać komentarze.
JavaScript jest wyłączony w Twojej przeglądarce internetowej. Włącz go, a następnie odśwież stronę, aby móc w pełni z niej korzystać.