PL EN


Preferencje help
Widoczny [Schowaj] Abstrakt
Liczba wyników
Czasopismo
2024 | 23 | nr 1 | 47--65
Tytuł artykułu

Grammatical Mood and Assessment of Support Policy Effectiveness

Warianty tytułu
Języki publikacji
EN
Abstrakty
EN
Motivation: The COVID-19 pandemic has had a significant impact on economic research, particularly in analyzing the effects of the virus on entire economies, specific sectors, and individual businesses. While there have been numerous studies on the pandemic's impact on the labor market, various economic sectors, and the environment, there remains a gap in research exploring the relationship between economics and linguistic aspects, such as language.

Aim: The purpose of this article is to address this research gap by examining the connection between language and economics. Specifically, the authors focus on the influence of linguistic variables on economic categories. In this study, the authors adopt the perspective that language can affect economic activities and aim to analyze how the grammatical form used in communication influences the assessment of the effectiveness of economic policy. By exploring this aspect, the article aims to contribute to a deeper understanding of the interplay between language and economics.

Results: Authors show that language may also be an essential framing instrument, while various grammatical moods affect the perception of messages that concern economic issues differently and are perceived differently depending on the level of identification with a given policy.(original abstract)
Słowa kluczowe
Czasopismo
Rocznik
Tom
23
Numer
Strony
47--65
Opis fizyczny
Twórcy
  • University of Gdańsk
  • University of Gdańsk
Bibliografia
  • Ai, Ch., & Norton, E.C. (2003). Interaction terms in logit and probit models. Economics Letters, 80(1), 123-129. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0165-1765(03)00032-6.
  • Atkeson, A. (2020). What will be the economic impact of COVID-19 in the US: rough estimates of disease scenarios. NBER Working Paper Series, 26867, 1-25. https://doi.org/10.3386/w26867.
  • Baker, T., & Judge, K. (2020). How to help small businesses survive COVID-19. Columbia Law and Economics Working Paper, 620, 1-12. https://doi.org/10.2139/ssrn.3571460.
  • Belenky, D.M., & Nokes-Malach, T.J. (2016). Mastery-approach goals and knowledge transfer: an investigation into the effects of task structure and framing instructions. Learning and Individual Differences, 25, 21-34. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2013.02.004.
  • Bernhofer, J., Costantini, F., & Kovacic, M. (2019). Risk Attitudes, Investment Behavior and Linguistic Variation. University Ca' Foscari of Venice, Department of Economics Research Paper Series, 34/15, 1-50. http://dx.doi.org/10.2139/ssrn.2708465.
  • Bizon, W. (2018), "Tell them it's easy": framing incentives in learning basic statistical problems. Journal of Behavioral and Experimental Economics, 76, 76-81. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socec.2018.08.001.
  • Bonifazi, G., Corradini, E., Ursino, D., & Virgili, L. (2022). New approaches to extract information from posts on COVID-19 published on Reddit. International Journal of Information Technology & Decision Making, 21(05), 1385-1431. https://doi.org/10.1142/s0219622022500213.
  • Bourgeois-Gironde, S., Mari, A., Nicolas, D., & Blunier, D. (2019). Grammatical mood and ambiguity aversion. In A. Botinis (Ed.), Proceedings of 10th International Conference of Experimental Linguistics (pp. 33-36). Centro de Linguística da Universidade de Lisboa. https://doi.org/10.36505/exling-2019/10/0008/000370.
  • Brückner, S., Förster, M., Zlatkin-Troitschanskaia, O., & Walstad, W.B. (2015). Effects of prior economic education, native language, and gender on economic knowledge of first-year students in higher education: a comparative study between Germany and the USA. Studies in Higher Education, 20(3), 437-453. https://doi.org/10.1080/03075079.2015.1004235.
  • Brülhart, M., Lalive, R., Lehmann, T., & Siegenthaler, M. (2020). COVID-19 financial support to small businesses in Switzerland: evaluation and outlook. Swiss Journal of Economics and Statistics, 156, 15. https://doi.org/10.1186/s41937-020-00060-y.
  • Chang, O.H., Nichols, D.R., & Schultz, J.J. (1987). Taxpayer attitudes toward tax audit risk. Journal of Economic Psychology, 8(3), 299-309. https://doi.org/10.1016/0167-4870(87)90025-0.
  • Chen, J.I., He, T-S., & Riyanto, Y.E. (2019). The effect of language on economic behavior: examining the causal link between future tense and time preference in the lab. European Economic Review, 120, 103307. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.euroecorev.2019.103307.
  • Chen, M.K. (2013). The effect of language on economic behavior: evidence from savings rates, health behaviors, and retirement assets. American Economic Review, 103(2), 690-731. https://doi.org/10.2139/ssrn.1914379.
  • Costa, A., Foucart, A., Arnon, I., Aparici, M., & Apesteguia, J. (2014). "Piensa" twice: on the foreign language effect in decision making. Cognition, 130(2), 236-254. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2013.11.010.
  • Cowen, T. (2019). Big business: a love letter to an American anti-hero. St. Martin's Press.
  • Davis, J.J. (1995). The effects of message framing on response to environmental communications. Journalism and Mass Communication Quarterly, 72(2), 285-299. https://doi.org/10.1177/107769909507200203.
  • Fairlie, R.W. (2020). The impact of COVID-19 on small business owners: evidence of early-stage losses from the April 2020 current population survey. NBER Working Paper Series, 27309, 1-21. https://doi.org/10.3386/w27309.
  • Faria-e-Castro, M. (2021). Fiscal policy during a pandemic. Journal of Economic Dynamics and Control, 125, 104088. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jedc.2021.104088.
  • Goodhart, Ch.A.E., Tsomocos, D.P., & Wang, X. (2023). Support for small businesses amid COVID-19. Economica, 90(358), 612-652. https://doi.org/10.1111/ecca.12460.
  • Graham, J.R., Harvey, C.R., & Huang, H. (2009). Investor competence, trading frequency, and home bias. Management Science, 55(7), 1094-1106. https://doi.org/10.3386/w11426.
  • Hagedorn, M., & Mitman, K. (2020). Corona policy according to hank. CEPR Discussion Paper, 14694.
  • Hambleton, R.K. (2005). Issues, designs, and technical guildness for adapting test into multiple languages. In R.K. Hambleton, P.F. Merenda, & C.D. Spielberger (Eds.), Adapting educational and psychological tests for cross-cultural assessment (pp. 3-38). Psychology Press. https://doi.org/10.4324/9781410611758-6.
  • Hansen, B. (2010). Mood in Polish. In B. Rothstein, & R. Thieroff (Eds.), Mood in the languages of Europe (pp. 342-357). John Benjamins. https://doi.org/10.1075/slcs.120.19han.
  • Hayakawa, S., & Keysar, B. (2018). Using a foreign language reduces mental imagery. Cognition, 173, 8-15. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2017.12.010.
  • Hayakawa, S., Lau, B.K.Y., Holtzmann, S., Costa, A., & Keysar, B. (2019). On the reliability of the foreign language effect on risk-taking. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 72(1), 29-40. https://doi.org/10.1177/1747021817742242.
  • He, T-S. (2017). "I" make you risk-averse: the effect of first-person pronoun use in a lottery choice experiment. Economics Letters, 153, 39-42. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.econlet.2017.01.014.
  • Hepburn, C., O'Callaghan, B., Stern, N., Stiglitz, J., & Zenghelis, D. (2020). Will COVID-19 fiscal recovery packages accelerate or retard progress on climate change. Oxford Review of Economic Policy, 36(Supplement_1), S359-S381. https://doi.org/10.1093/oxrep/graa015.
  • Holst, H., Fessler, A., & Niehoff, S. (2021). COVID-19, social class and work experience in Germany: inequalities in work-related health and economic risks. European Societies, 23(sup1), S495-S512. https://doi.org/10.1080/14616696.2020.1828979.
  • Hunter, S.T., Bedell-Avers, K.E., & Mumford, M.D. (2009). Impact of situational framing and complexity on charismatic, ideological and pragmatic leaders: investigation using a computer simulation. The Leadership Quarterly, 20(3), 383-40. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.leaqua.2009.03.007.
  • Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1979). Prospect theory: an analysis of decision under risk. Econometrica, 47(2), 263-291. https://doi.org/10.2307/1914185.
  • Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1984). Choices, values, and frames. American Psychologist, 39(4), 341-350. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066x.39.4.341.
  • Keysar, B., Hayakawa, S.L., & An, S.G. (2012). The foreign-language effect: thinking in a foreign tongue reduces decision biases. Psychological Science, 23(6), 661-668. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797611432178.
  • Krzysztofik, R., Kantor-Pietraga, I., & Spórna, T. (2020). Spatial and functional dimensions of the COVID-19 epidemic in Poland. Eurasian Geography and Economics, 61(4-5), 573-586. https://doi.org/10.1080/15387216.2020.1783337.
  • Lavecchia, A.M., Liu, H., & Oreopoulos, P. (2014). Behavioral economics of education: progress and possibilities. NBER Working Paper Series, 20609, 1-90. https://doi.org/10.3386/w20609.
  • Levitt, S.D., List, J.A., Neckermann, S., & Sadoff, S. (2012). The behavioralist goes to school: leveraging behavioral economics to improve educational performance. NBER Working Paper Series, 18165, 1-48. https://doi.org/10.3386/w18165.
  • McFarlane, S., Cipoletti-Perez, H., & Weissglass C. (2020). Thinking in a non-native language: a new nudge. Frontierrs in Psychology, 11, 549083. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.549083.
  • Mecit, A., Shrum, L.J., & Lowrey, T.M. (2022). COVID-19 is feminine: grammatical gender influences danger perceptions and precautionary behavioral intentions by activating gender stereotypes. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 32(2), 316-325. https://doi.org/10.1002/jcpy.1257.
  • Miller, M.R. (2021). Language choice about COVID-19 vaccines can save lives. Journal of Communication in Healthcare, 14(2), 99-101. https://doi.org/10.1080/17538068.2021.1892285.
  • Mweri, J. (2021). Corona Virus Disease (COVID-19) effects on language use: an analysis of neologisms. Linguistics and Literature Studies, 9(1), 36-47. https://doi.org/10.13189/lls.2021.090105.
  • Oehler, A., Wendt, S., & Horn, M. (2017). Are investors really home-biased when investing at home. Research in International Business and Finance, 40, 52-60. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ribaf.2016.12.003.
  • Oyebode, O., Ndulue, C., Mulchandani, D., Suruliraj, B., Adib, A., Orji, F.A., Milios, E., Matwin, S., & Orji, R. (2022). COVID-19 pandemic: identifying key issues using social media and natural language processing. Journal of Healthcare Informatics Research, 6, 174-207. https://doi.org/10.1007/s41666-021-00111-w.
  • Ozili, P.K., & Arun, T. (2020). Spillover of COVID-19: impact on the global economy. MPRA Paper, 998500, 1-27.
  • Park, J., Kim, J., Lee, D.C., Kim, S.S., Voyer, B.G., Kim, C., & Yoon, S. (2022). The impact of COVID-19 on consumer evaluation of authentic advertising messages. Psychology & Marketing, 39(1), 76-89. https://doi.org/10.1002/mar.21574.
  • Penn, J.M. (2014). Linguistic relativity versus innate ideas: the origins of the Sapir-Whorf hypothesis in German thought. de Gruyter. https://doi.org/10.1515/9783110818444.
  • Peters, E., Västfjäll, D., Slovic, P., Mertz, C.K., Mazzocco, K., & Dickert, S. (2006). Numeracy and decision making. Psychological Science, 17(5), 407-413. https://doi.org/10.1037/e640112011-034.
  • Piekkari, R., Tietze, S., Angouri, J., Meyer, R., & Vaara, E. (2021). Can you speak COVID-19: languages and social inequality in management studies. Journal of Management Studies, 58(2), 587-591. https://doi.org/10.1111/joms.12657.
  • Ribeiro-Soriano, D. (2017). Small business and entrepreneurship: their role in economic and social development. Entrepreneurship & Regional Development, 29(1-2), 1-3. https://doi.org/10.1080/08985626.2016.1255438.
  • Roberts, S.G., Winters, J., & Chen, K. (2015). Future tense and economic decisions: controlling for cultural evolution. PLoS ONE, 10(7), e0132145. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0132145.
  • Sapir, E. (1921). An introduction to the study of speech. Brace & World.
  • Stam, J.H. (1980). An historical perspective on 'linguistic relativity'. In R.W. Rieber (Ed.), Psychology of language and thought (pp. 239-262). Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4684-3644-0_11.
  • Sutter, M., Angerer, S., Glätzle-Rützler, D., & Lergetporer, P. (2018). Language group differences in time preferences: evidence from primary school children in a bilingual city. European Economic Review, 106, 21-34. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.euroecorev.2018.04.003.
  • Thieroff, R. (2010). Moods, moods, moods. In B. Rothstein, & R. Thieroff (Eds.), Mood in the languages of Europe (pp. 1-30). John Benjamins. https://doi.org/10.1075/slcs.120.01thi.
  • van de Ridder, J.M.M., Peters, C.M.M., Stokking, K.M., de Ru, J.A., & ten Cate, O.T.J. (2015). Framing of feedback impacts student's satisfaction, self-efficacy and performance. Advances in Health Sciences Education, 20(3), 803-816. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10459-014-9567-8.
  • van de Vijver, F.J.R., & Leung, K. (2000). Methodological issues in psychological research on culture. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 31(1), 33-51. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022022100031001004.
  • Vansteenkiste, M., Matos, L., Lens, W., & Soenens, B. (2007). Understanding the impact of intrinsic versus extrinsic goal framing on exercise performance: the conflicting role of task and ego involvement. Psychology of Sport & Exercise, 8(5), 771-794. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychsport.2006.04.006.
  • Vegt, I.V.D., & Kleinberg, B. (2020). Women worry about family, men about the economy: gender differences in emotional responses to COVID-19. In S. Aref, K. Bontcheva, M. Braghieri, F. Dignum, F. Giannotti, F. Grisolia, & D. Pedreschi (Eds.), International conference on social informatics (pp. 397-409). Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-60975-7_29.
  • Verma, S., & Gustafsson, A. (2020). Investigating the emerging COVID-19 research trends in the field of business and management: a bibliometric analysis approach. Journal of Business Research, 118, 253-261. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbusres.2020.06.057.
  • Vives, M-L, Aparici, M., & Costa, A. (2018). The limits of the foreign language effect on decision-making: the case of the outcome bias and the representativeness heuristic. PLoS ONE, 13(9), e0203528. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0203528.
  • Whorf, B.L., & Caroll, J.B. (1964). Language, thought, and reality: selected writings of Benjamin Lee Whorf. MIT Press.
  • Yadav, A., & Vishwakarma, D.K. (2021). A language-independent network to analyze the impact of COVID-19 on the world via sentiment analysis. ACM Transactions on Internet Technology, 22(1), 1-30. https://doi.org/10.1145/3475867.
  • Zaremba, A., Kizys, R., Aharon, D.Y., & Demir, E. (2020). Infected markets: novel coronavirus, government interventions, and stock return volatility around the globe. Finance Research Letters, 35, 101597. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.frl.2020.101597.
Typ dokumentu
Bibliografia
Identyfikator YADDA
bwmeta1.element.ekon-element-000171688032

Zgłoszenie zostało wysłane

Zgłoszenie zostało wysłane

Musisz być zalogowany aby pisać komentarze.
JavaScript jest wyłączony w Twojej przeglądarce internetowej. Włącz go, a następnie odśwież stronę, aby móc w pełni z niej korzystać.