PL EN


Preferencje help
Widoczny [Schowaj] Abstrakt
Liczba wyników
Czasopismo
2015 | 4(4) | 23--38
Tytuł artykułu

Neurobiology of Consciousness : Current Research and Perspectives

Autorzy
Treść / Zawartość
Warianty tytułu
Języki publikacji
EN
Abstrakty
EN
Scientific, objective approach to consciousness has allowed to obtain some experimental data concerning brain activity, ignoring, however, the longstanding philosophical tradition. Spectacular development of neuroscience which has been observed recently made this dissonance particularly noticeable. The paper addresses the main problems of discrepancy between neurobiological research and philosophical perspective. Current opinions concerning neural correlates and models of consciousness are discussed, as well as the problems of working memory, attention, self, and disorders of consciousness. A new neurobiological approach to describe brain function in terms of brain connectivity (so-called connectome) is also presented. Finally, the need to introduce at least some aspects of philosophical approach directly into neurobiological research of consciousness is postulated. (original abstract)
Czasopismo
Rocznik
Tom
Strony
23--38
Opis fizyczny
Twórcy
  • Jagiellonian University, Kraków
Bibliografia
  • 1. Abraham, A., "The world according to me: personal relevance and the medial prefrontal cortex", Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 7, 2013, pp. 341.
  • 2. Allen, A.K., Wilkins, K., Gazzaley, A., Morsella, E., "Conscious thoughts from reflex-like processes: a new experimental paradigm for consciousness research", Consciousness and Cognition 22(4), 2013, pp. 1318-1331.
  • 3. Amico, E., Gomez, F., Di Perri, C., Vanhaudenhuyse, A., Lesenfants, D., Boveroux, P., Bonhomme, V., Brichant, J.F., Marinazzo, D., Laureys, S., "Posterior cingulate cortex-related co-activation patterns: a resting state FMRI study in propofol-induced loss of consciousness", PLoS One. 9(6), 2014, pp. e100012.
  • 4. Andrews-Hanna, J.R., Smallwood, J., Spreng, R.N., "The default network and selfgenerated thought: component processes, dynamic control, and clinical relevance", Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 1316, 2014, pp. 29-52.
  • 5. Arroyo-Anlló, E.M., Bouston, A.T., Fargeau, M.N., Orgaz Baz, B., Gil, R., "SelfConsciousness in Patients with Behavioral Variant Frontotemporal Dementia", Journal of Alzheimer's Disease 2015 Nov 23. [Epub ahead of print]
  • 6. Aru, J., Bachmann, T., "Phenomenal awareness can emerge without attention", Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 7, 2013, pp. 891.
  • 7. Aru, J., Bachmann, T., "Still wanted - the mechanisms of consciousness!", Frontiers in Psychology 6, 2015, pp. 5.
  • 8. Aspell, J.E., Lenggenhager, B., Blanke, O., "Keeping in touch with one's self: multisensory mechanisms of self-consciousness", PLoS One 4(8), 2009, pp. e6488.
  • 9. Baars, B.J., A cognitive theory of consciousness. New York: Cambridge University Press, 1988.
  • 10. Baars, B.J., In the theater of consciousness. New York: Oxford University Press, 1997.
  • 11. Bachmann, T., "Attention as a process of selection, perception as a process of representation, and phenomenal experience as the resulting process of perception being modulated by a dedicated consciousness mechanism", Frontiers in Psychology 2, 2011, pp. 387.
  • 12. Bachmann, T., "How to begin to overcome the ambiguity present in differentiation between contents and levels of consciousness?", Frontiers in Psychology 3, 2012, pp. 82.
  • 13. Bachmann, T., Hudetz, A.G., "It is time to combine the two main traditions in the research on the neural correlates of consciousness: C = L × D", Frontiers in Psychology 5, 2014, pp. 940.
  • 14. Baddeley, A.D., Allen, R.J., Hitch, G.J., "Binding in visual working memory: the role of the episodic buffer", Neuropsychologia 49, 2011, pp. 1393-1400.
  • 15. Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A., Frith, U., "Does the autistic child have a "theory of mind"?", Cognition 21(1), 1985, pp. 37-46.
  • 16. Bayne, T., "Self-consciousness and the Unity of Consciousness", Monist 87 (2), 2004, pp. 219-236.
  • 17. Berkovich-Ohana, A., Glicksohn, J., "The consciousness state space (CSS)-a unifying model for consciousness and self", Frontiers in Psychology 5, 2014, pp. 341.
  • 18. Blanke, O., "Multisensory brain mechanisms of bodily self-consciousness", Nature Reviews Neuroscience 13(8), 2012, pp. 556-571.
  • 19. Blanke, O., Metzinger, T., "Full-body illusions and minimal phenomenal selfhood", Trends in Cognitive Sciences 13(1), 2009, pp. 7-13.
  • 20. Blanke, O., Slater, M., Serino, A., "Behavioral, neural, and computational principles of bodily self-consciousness", Neuron 88(1), 2015, pp. 145-166.
  • 21. Block, N., "On a confusion about a function of consciousness", Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18, 1995, pp. 227-247.
  • 22. Block, N., "How can we find the neural correlate of consciousness?", Trends in Neuroscience 19(11), 1996, pp. 456-9.
  • 23. Block, N., "Two neural correlates of consciousness", Trends in Cognitive Sciences 9(2), 2005, pp. 46-52.
  • 24. Bonfiglio, L., Piarulli, A., Olcese, U., Andre, P., Arrighi, P., Frisoli, A., Rossi, B., Bergamasco, M., Carboncini, M.C., "Spectral parameters modulation and source localization of blink-related alpha and low-beta oscillations differentiate minimally conscious state from vegetative state/unresponsive wakefulness syndrome", PLoS One. 9(3), 2014, pp. e93252. Erratum in: PLoS One. 9(4), pp. e95948.
  • 25. Brewer, J.A., Garrison, K.A., Whitfield-Gabrieli, S., "What about the "self" is processed in the posterior cingulate cortex?", Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 7, 2013, pp. 647.
  • 26. Christoff, K., Cosmelli, D., Legrand, D., Thompson, E., "Specifying the self for cognitive neuroscience", Trends in Cognitive Science 15(3), 2011, pp. 104-112.
  • 27. Cohen, M.A., Konkle, T., Rhee, J.Y., Nakayama, K., Alvarez G.A., "Processing multiple visual objects is limited by overlap in neural channels", Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 111(24), 2014, pp. 8955-60.
  • 28. Cowan, N., "Multiple concurrent thoughts: The meaning and developmental neuropsychology of working memory", Developmental Neuropsychology 35(5), 2010, pp. 447-474.
  • 29. Crick F., Koch C., "Towards a neurobiological theory of consciousness", Seminars in the Neurosciences 2, 1990, pp. 263-275.
  • 30. Crick, F., Koch, C., "Consciousness and neuroscience", Cerebral Cortex 8(2), 1998, pp. 97-107.
  • 31. D'Argembeau, A., Ruby, P., Collette, F., Degueldre, C., Balteau, E., Luxen, A., Maquet, P., Salmon, E., "Distinct regions of the medial prefrontal cortex are associated with selfreferential processing and perspective taking", Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 19(6), 2007, pp. 935-44.
  • 32. D'Argembeau, A., Feyers, D., Majerus, S., Collette, F., Van der Linden, M., Maquet, P., Salmon, E., "Self-reflection across time: cortical midline structures differentiate between present and past selves", Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 3(3), 2008, pp. 244-25.
  • 33. D'Argembeau, A., Cassol, H., Phillips, C., Balteau, E., Salmon, E., Van der Linden, M., "Brains creating stories of selves: the neural basis of autobiographical reasoning", Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 9(5), 2014, pp. 646-52.
  • 34. DaSilveira, A., DeSouza, M.L., Gomes, W.B., "Self-consciousness concept and assessment in self-report measures", Frontiers in Psychology 6, 2015, pp. 930.
  • 35. Davis, J.I., Markman, A.B., "Embodied cognition as a practical paradigm: introduction to the topic, the future of embodied cognition", Topics in Cognitive Science 4(4), 2012, pp. 685-91.
  • 36. Dehaene, S., Sergent, C., Changeux, J.-P., "A neuronal network model linking subjective reports and objective physiological data during conscious perception", Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 100(14), 2003, pp. 8520-8525.
  • 37. Demertzi, A., Gómez, F., Crone, J.S., Vanhaudenhuyse, A., Tshibanda, L., Noirhomme, Q., Thonnard, M., Charland-Verville, V., Kirsch, M., Laureys, S., Soddu, A., "Multiple fMRI system-level baseline connectivity is disrupted in patients with consciousness alterations", Cortex 52, 2014, pp. 35-46.
  • 38. Demertzi, A., Antonopoulos, G., Heine, L., Voss, H.U., Crone, J.S., de Los Angeles, C., Bahri, M.A., Di Perri, C., Vanhaudenhuyse, A., Charland-Verville, V., Kronbichler, M., Trinka, E., Phillips, C., Gomez, F., Tshibanda, L., Soddu, A., Schiff, N.D., Whitfield-Gabrieli, S., Laureys, S., "Intrinsic functional connectivity differentiates minimally conscious from unresponsive patients", Brain 138(Pt 9), 2015, pp. 2619-31.
  • 39. De Sousa, A., "Towards an integrative theory of consciousness: part 1 (neurobiological and cognitive models)", Mens Sana Monographs 11(1), 2013, pp. 100-150.
  • 40. De Sousa, A., "Towards an integrative theory of consciousness: part 2 (an anthology of various other models)", Mens Sana Monographs 11(1), 2013, pp. 151-209.
  • 41. Di Perri, C., Thibaut, A., Heine, L., Soddu, A., Demertzi, A., Laureys, S., "Measuring consciousness in coma and related states", World Journal of Radiology 6(8), 2014, pp. 589-97.
  • 42. Edelman, G.M., Gally, J.A., Baars, J., "Biology of consciousness", Frontiers in Psychology 2, 2011, pp. 4.
  • 43. Fabbro, F., Aglioti, S.M., Bergamasco, M., Clarici, A., Panksepp, J., "Evolutionary aspects of self- and world consciousness in vertebrates", Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 9, 2015, pp. 157.
  • 44. Faivre N, Salomon R, Blanke O., "Visual consciousness and bodily selfconsciousness", Current Opinions in Neurology 28(1), 2015, pp. 23-28.
  • 45. Feinberg, T.E., "Neuroontology, neurobiological naturalism, and consciousness: A challenge to scientific reduction and a solution", Physics of Life Reviews 9, 2012, pp. 13-34.
  • 46. Flagan, T., Beer, J.S., "Three ways in which midline regions contribute to selfevaluation", Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 7, 2013, pp. 45.
  • 47. Foster, P.P., "Role of physical and mental training in brain network configuration", Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience 7, 2015, pp. 117.
  • 48. Gallagher, S., "Philosophical conceptions of the self: implications for cognitive science", Trends in Cognitive Science 4(1), 2000, pp. 14-21.
  • 49. Gallagher, S., "A pattern theory of self", Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 7, 2013, pp. 443.
  • 50. Gallese, V., Lakoff, G., "The brain's concepts: the role of the sensory-motor system in conceptual knowledge", Cognitive Neuroscience 22(3/4), 2005, pp. 455-479.
  • 51. Gardiner, J.M., "Episodic memory and autonoetic consciousness: a first-person approach", Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences 356(1413), 2001, pp. 1351-1361.
  • 52. Gierer, A., "Brain, mind and limitations of a scientific theory of human consciousness", BioEssays 30, 2008, pp. 499-505.
  • 53. Gil, R., Arroyo-Anllo, E.M., Ingrand, P., Gil, M., Neau, J.P., Ornon, C., Bonnaud, V., "Self-consciousness and Alzheimer's disease", Acta Neurologica Scandinavica 104(5), 2001, pp. 296-300.
  • 54. Gosseries, O., Schnakers, C., Ledoux, D., Vanhaudenhuyse, A., Bruno, M.A., Demertzi, A., Noirhomme, Q., Lehembre, R., Damas, P., Goldman, S., Peeters, E., Moonen, G., Laureys, S., "Automated EEG entropy measurements in coma, vegetative state/unresponsive wakefulness syndrome and minimally conscious state", Functional Neurology 26(1), 2011, 25-30.
  • 55. Guldenmund, P., Stender, J., Heine, L., Laureys, S., "Mindsight: diagnostics in disorders of consciousness", Critical Care Research and Practice 2012, 2012, pp. 624724.
  • 56. Guldenmund, P., Vanhaudenhuyse, A., Boly, M., Laureys, S., Soddu, A.., "A default mode of brain function in altered states of consciousness", Archives Italiennes de Biologie 150, 2012, pp. 107-21.
  • 57. Jacobs, C. Silvanto, J., "How is working memory content consciously experienced? The 'conscious copy' model of WM introspection", Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews 55, 2015, pp. 510-519.
  • 58. Kanwisher, N., "Functional specificity in the human brain: a window into the functional architecture of the mind", Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 107(25), 2010, pp. 11163-1170.
  • 59. Keysers, C., Gazzola, V., "Expanding the mirror: vicarious activity for actions, emotions, and sensations", Current Opinions in Neurobiology 19(6), 2009, pp. 666-671.
  • 60. Kiefer, M., Pulvermüller, F., "Conceptual representations in mind and brain: theoretical developments, current evidence and future directions", Cortex 48(7), 2012, pp. 805-25.
  • 61. Kinsbourne, M., A continuum of self-consciousness that emerges in phylogeny and ontogeny [in:] Herbert S. Terrace and Janet Metcalfe (eds.) The Missing Link in Cognition: Origins of Self-Reflective Consciousness, Oxford University Press, New York, NY, USA, 2005.
  • 62. Kiverstein, J., Miller, M., "The embodied brain: towards a radical embodied cognitive neuroscience", Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 9, 2015, pp. 237.
  • 63. Klein, S.B., "Making the case that episodic recollection is attributable to operations occurring at retrieval rather than to content stored in a dedicated subsystem of long-term memory", Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience 7, 2013, pp. 3.
  • 64. Klein, S.B., Gangi, C.E., "The multiplicity of self: neuropsychological evidence and its implications for the self as a construct in psychological research", Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 1191, 2010, pp. 1-15.
  • 65. Koch, Ch., Tsuchiya, N., "Attention and consciousness: two distinct brain processes", Trends in Cognitive Sciences 11(1), 2007, pp. 16-22.
  • 66. Kriegel, U., "Consciousness and Self-Consciousness", Monist 87(2), 2004, pp. 182- 205.
  • 67. Lamme, V.A., "Why visual attention and awareness are different", Trends in Cognitive Sciences 7(1), 2003, pp. 12-18.
  • 68. Lamme, V.A., Roelfsema, P., "The distinct modes of vision offered by feedforward and recurrent processing", Trends in Neuroscience 23(11), 2000, pp. 571-579.
  • 69. Lau, H., Rosenthal, D., "Empirical support for higher-order theories of conscious awareness", Trends in Cognitive Sciences 15(8), 2011, pp. 365-73.
  • 70. Laureys, S., "The neural correlate of (un)awareness: Lessons from the vegetative state", Trends in Cognitive Sciences 9(12), 2005, pp. 556-559.
  • 71. Lenggenhager, B., Tadi, T., Metzinger, T., Blanke, O., "Video ergo sum: manipulating bodily self-consciousness", Science 317(5841), 2007, pp. 1096-1099.
  • 72. Lepsien, J., Nobre, A.C., "Cognitive control of attention in the human brain: insights from orienting attention to mental representations", Brain Research 1105(1), 2006, pp. 20-31.
  • 73. Lombardo, M.V., Chakrabarti, B., Bullmore, E.T.; MRC AIMS Consortium, BaronCohen, S., "Specialization of right temporo-parietal junction for mentalizing and its relation to social impairments in autism", Neuroimage 56(3), 2011, pp. 1832-8.
  • 74. Lombardo, M.V., Chakrabarti, B., Bullmore, E.T., Wheelwright, S.J., Sadek, S.A., Suckling, J.; MRC AIMS Consortium, Baron-Cohen, S., "Shared neural circuits for mentalizing about the self and others", Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 22(7), 2009, pp. 1623-1635.
  • 75. Mahon, B.Z., Caramazza, A., "A critical look at the embodied cognition hypothesis and a new proposal for grounding conceptual content", Journal of Physiology-Paris. 102(1-3), 2008, pp. 59-70.
  • 76. Markowitsch, H.J., "Memory and self-neuroscientific landscapes", International Scholarly Research Notices Neuroscience 2013, 2013, pp. 176027.
  • 77. Markowitsch HJ, Staniloiu A., "Memory, autonoetic consciousness, and the self", Consciousness and Cognition 20(1), 2011, pp. 16-39.
  • 78. Mason, M.F., Norton, M.I., Van Horn, J.D., Wegner, D.M., Grafton, S.T., Macrae, C.N.,, "Wandering minds: the default network and stimulus-independent thought", Science 315(5810), 2007, pp. 393-395.
  • 79. Mehta, N., Mashour, G.A., "General and specific consciousness: a first-order representationalist approach", Frontiers in Psychology 4, 2013, pp. 407.
  • 80. Menon, V., Uddin, L.Q., "Saliency, switching, attention and control: a network model of insula function", Brain Structure and Function 214(5-6), 2010, pp. 655-667.
  • 81. Metzinger, T., Being no one. The self-model theory of subjectivity, Cambridge: MIT Press, 2003.
  • 82. Metzinger, T., "Empirical perspectives from the self-model theory of subjectivity: a brief summary with examples", Progress in Brain Research 168, 2008, pp. 215-245.
  • 83. Miller, S.M., "Closing in on the constitution of consciousness", Frontiers in Psychology 5, 2014, pp. 1293.
  • 84. Molnar-Szakacs, I., Uddin, L.Q., "Self-processing and the default mode network: interactions with the mirror neuron system", Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 7, 2013, pp. 571.
  • 85. Moran, J.M., Kelley, W.M., Heatherton, T.F., "What can the organization of the brain's default mode network tell us about self-knowledge", Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 7, 2013, pp. 391.
  • 86. Musholt, K., "A philosophical perspective on the relation between cortical midline structures and the self", Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 7, 2013, pp. 536.
  • 87. Neisser, U., "Five kinds of self-knowledge". Philosophical Psychology 1, 1988, pp. 35-59.
  • 88. Nelson, K., Emerging Levels of Consciousness in Early Human Development [in:] Herbert S. Terrace and Janet Metcalfe (eds.) The Missing Link in Cognition: Origins of SelfReflective Consciousness, Oxford University Press, New York, NY, USA, 2005.
  • 89. Nelson, S.M., Dosenbach, N.U., Cohen, A.L., Wheeler, M.E., Schlaggar, B.L., Petersen, S.E., "Role of the anterior insula in task-level control and focal attention", Brain Structure and Function 214(5-6), 2010, pp. 669-680.
  • 90. Noel, J.P., Pfeiffer, C., Blanke, O., Serino, A., "Peripersonal space as the space of the bodily self", Cognition 144, 2015, pp. 49-57.
  • 91. Northoff, G., "From emotions to consciousness - a neuro-phenomenal and neurorelational approach", Frontiers in Psychology 3, 2012, pp. 303.
  • 92. Northoff, G., "Brain and self - a neurophilosophical account", Child and Adolescent Psychiatry and Mental Health 7, 2013, pp. 28.
  • 93. Northoff, G., Bermpohl, F., "Cortical midline structures and the self", Trends in Cognitive Sciences 8(3), 2004, pp. 102-107.
  • 94. Olivé, I., Tempelmann, C., Berthoz, A., Heinze, H.J., "Increased functional connectivity between superior colliculus and brain regions implicated in bodily self-consciousness during the rubber hand illusion", Human Brain Mapping 36(2), 2015, 717-730.
  • 95. Panksepp, J., Northoff, G., "The trans-species core SELF: the emergence of active cultural and neuro-ecological agents through self-related processing within subcortical-cortical midline networks", Consciousness and Cognition 18(1), 2009, pp. 193-215.
  • 96. Parks, E.L., Madden, D.J., "Brain connectivity and visual attention", Brain Connectivity 3(4), 2013, pp. 317-38.
  • 97. Perlovsky, L., "Language and cognition interaction neural mechanisms", Computational Intelligence and Neuroscience 2011, 2011, pp. 454587.
  • 98. Perlovsky, L., Ilin, R., "Neurally and mathematically motivated architecture for language and thought, The Open Neuroimaging Journal 4, 2010, pp. 70-80.
  • 99. Pessoa, L., "Understanding brain networks and brain organization", Physics of Life Reviews 11(3), 2014, pp. 400-35.
  • 100. Pfeiffer, C., Serino, A., Blanke, O., "The vestibular system: a spatial reference for bodily self-consciousness", Frontiers in Integrative Neuroscience 8, 2014, pp. 31.
  • 101. Posner, M.I., Rothbart, M.K., "Toward a physical basis of attention and self regulation", Physics of Life Reviews 6(2), 2009, pp. 103-129.
  • 102. Qin, P., Northoff, G., "How is our self related to midline regions and the defaultmode network?", Neuroimage 57, 2011, pp. 1221-1233.
  • 103. Qin, P., Wu, X., Huang, Z., Duncan, N.W., Tang, W., Wolff, A., Hu, J., Gao, L., Jin, Y., Wu, X., Zhang, J., Lu, L., Wu, C., Qu, X., Mao, Y., Weng, X., Zhang, J., Northoff, G., "How are different neural networks related to consciousness?", Annals of Neurology 78(4), 2015, pp. 594- 605.
  • 104. Raffone, A., Pantani, M., "A global workspace model for phenomenal and access consciousness", Consciousness and Cognition 19(2), 2010, pp. 580-96.
  • 105. Raffone, A., Srinivasan, N., van Leeuwen, C., "The interplay of attention and consciousness in visual search, attentional blink and working memory consolidation", Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B 369(1641), 2014, pp. 20130215.
  • 106. Raffone, A., Srinivasan, N., van Leeuwen, C., "Rapid switching and complementary evidence accumulation enable flexibility of an all-or-none global workspace for control of attentional and conscious processing: a reply to Wyble et al", Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B 370(1661), 2015, pp. 20140315.
  • 107. Raichle, M.E., MacLeod, A.M., Snyder, A.Z., Powers, W.J., Gusnard, D.A., Shulman, G.L., "A default mode of brain function", Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 98(2), 2001, pp. 676-82.
  • 108. Rosenthal, D., "Higher-order awareness, misrepresentation and function", Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B 367(1594), 2012, pp. 1424-38.
  • 109. Sala-Llonch, R., Bartres-Faz, D., Junqué C., "Reorganization of brain networks in aging: a review of functional connectivity studies", Frontiers in Psychology 6, 2015, pp. 663.
  • 110. Sandrone, S., "Self through the mirror (neurons) and default mode network: what neuroscientists found and what can still be found there", Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 7, 2013, pp. 383.
  • 111. Schendan, H.E., Ganis, G., "Electrophysiological potentials reveal cortical mechanisms for mental imagery, mental simulation, and grounded (embodied) cognition", Frontiers in Psychology 3, 2012, pp. 329.
  • 112. Serino, A., Alsmith, A., Costantini, M., Mandrigin, A., Tajadura-Jimenez, A., Lopez, C., "Bodily ownership and self-location: components of bodily self-consciousness", Consciousness and Cognition 22(4), 2013, pp. 1239-1252.
  • 113. Shad, M.U., Keshavan, M.S., "Neurobiology of insight deficits in schizophrenia: an fMRI study", Schizophrenia Research 165(2-3), 2015, pp. 220-226.
  • 114. Shad, M.U., Keshavan, M.S., Steinberg, J.L., Mihalakos, P., Thomas, B.P., Motes, M.A., Soares, J.C., Tamminga, C.A., "Neurobiology of self-awareness in schizophrenia: an fMRI study", Schizophrenia Research 138(2-3), 2012, pp. 113-119.
  • 115. Singer, T., "The neuronal basis and ontogeny of empathy and mind reading: review of literature and implications for future research", Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews 30(6), 2006, pp. 855-863.
  • 116. Soto, D., Silvanto, J., "Reappraising the relationship between working memory and conscious awareness", Trends in Cognitive Sciences 18(10), 2014, pp. 520-525.
  • 117. Stawarczyk, D., Majerus, S., Maquet, P., D'Argembeau, A., "Neural correlates of ongoing conscious experience: both task-unrelatedness and stimulus-independence are related to default network activity", PLoS ONE 6(2), 2011, pp. e16997.
  • 118. Stevenson, R.J., "Phenomenal and access consciousness in olfaction", Consciousness and Cognition 18, 2009, pp. 1004-1017.
  • 119. Sugiura, M., "Associative account of self-cognition: extended forward model and multi-layer structure", Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 9, 2013, pp. 535.
  • 120. Symons, C.S., Johnson, B.T, "The self-reference effect in memory: a meta-analysis", Psychological Bulletin 121, 1997, pp. 371-394.
  • 121. Tagini, A., Raffone, A., "The 'I' and the 'Me' in self-referential awareness: a neurocognitive hypothesis", Cognitive Processing 11(1), 2010, pp. 9-20.
  • 122. Taya, F., Sun, Y., Babiloni, F., Thakor, N., Bezerianos, A., "Brain enhancement through cognitive training: a new insight from brain connectome", Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience 9, 2015, pp. 44.
  • 123. Tulving, E., Elements of episodic memory. New York: Oxford University Press, 1983.
  • 124. Tulving, E., "Memory and consciousness", Canadian Psychology 26, 1985, pp. 1-12.
  • 125. Tulving, E., "Episodic memory: from mind to brain", Annual Review of Psychology 53, 2002, pp. 1-25.
  • 126. Tulving E., Episodic memory and autonoesis: uniquely human? [in:] Herbert S. Terrace and Janet Metcalfe (eds.) The Missing Link in Cognition: Origins of Self-Reflective Consciousness, Oxford University Press, New York, NY, USA, 2005.
  • 127. van Boxtel, J.J., Tsuchiya, N., Koch, C., "Consciousness and attention: on sufficiency and necessity", Frontiers in Psychology 1, 2010, pp. 217.
  • 128. Vandekerckhove, M., Bulnes, L.C., Panksepp, J., "The emergence of primary anoetic consciousness in episodic memory", Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience 7, 2014, pp. 210.
  • 129. Vandekerckhove, M., Panksepp, J., "The flow of anoetic to noetic and autonoetic consciousness: a vision of unknowing (anoetic) and knowing (noetic) consciousness in the remembrance of things past and imagined futures", Consciousness and Cognition 18(4), 2009, pp. 1018-1028.
  • 130. Vandekerckhove, M., Panksepp, J., "A neurocognitive theory of higher mental emergence: from anoetic affective experiences to noetic knowledge and autonoetic awareness", Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews 35(9), 2011, pp. 2017-2025.
  • 131. van der Meer, L., Costafreda, S., Aleman, A., David, A.S., "Self-reflection and the brain: a theoretical review and meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies with implications for schizophrenia", Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews 34(6), 2010, pp. 935-946.
  • 132. Van Essen, D.C., Smith, S.M., Barch, D.M., Behrens, T.E., Yacoub, E., Ugurbil, K.; WU-Minn HCP Consortium, "The WU-Minn Human Connectome Project: an overview", Neuroimage 80, 2013, 62-79.
  • 133. von dem Hagen, E.A., Stoyanova, R.S., Rowe, J.B., Baron-Cohen, S., Calder, A.J., "Direct gaze elicits atypical activation of the theory-of-mind network in autism spectrum conditions", Cerebral Cortex 24(6), 2014, pp. 1485-92.
  • 134. Watanabe, M., "Are there internal thought processes in the monkey?--default brain activity in humans and nonhuman primates", Behavioural Brain Research 221(1), 2011, pp. 295- 303.
  • 135. Willems, R.M., Casasanto, D., "Flexibility in embodied language understanding", Frontiers in Psychology 2, 2011, pp. 116.
  • 136. Wu, X., Zou, Q., Hu, J., Tang, W., Mao, Y., Gao, L., Zhu, J., Jin, Y., Wu, X., Lu, L., Zhang, Y., Zhang, Y., Dai, Z., Gao, J.H., Weng, X., Zhou, L., Northoff, G., Giacino, J.T., He, Y., Yang, Y., "Intrinsic functional connectivity patterns predict consciousness level and recovery outcome in acquired brain injury", Journal of Neuroscience 35(37), 2015, pp. 12932-46.
  • 137. Wyble, B., Bowman, H., Nieuwenstein, M., "On the interplay between working memory consolidation and attentional selection in controlling conscious access: parallel processing at a cost--a comment on 'The interplay of attention and consciousness in visual search, attentional blink and working memory consolidation", Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 370(1661), 2015, pp. 20140197.
  • 138. Zahavi, D., Parnas, J., "Phenomenal consciousness and self-awareness: A phenomenological critique of representational theory", Journal of Consciousness Studies 5, 1998, pp. 687-705.
  • 139. Zhang, Y., Du, G., Yang, Y., Qin, W., Li, X., Zhang, Q., "Higher integrity of the motor and visual pathways in long-term video game players", Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 9, 2015, pp. 98.
Typ dokumentu
Bibliografia
Identyfikatory
Identyfikator YADDA
bwmeta1.element.ekon-element-000171420072

Zgłoszenie zostało wysłane

Zgłoszenie zostało wysłane

Musisz być zalogowany aby pisać komentarze.
JavaScript jest wyłączony w Twojej przeglądarce internetowej. Włącz go, a następnie odśwież stronę, aby móc w pełni z niej korzystać.